Articles
Please be aware that the below materials are protected by copyright.
Berent, I. (2025). Who is afraid of innate knowledge? Cerebral Cortex, 35(2).
Berent, I., & Hooley, J. M. (2025). The gender-sex incongruence is partly a mind–body incongruence. Scientific Reports, 15(1), 9185–13.
Berent, I., & Sansiveri, A. (2024). Davinci the Dualist: The Mind-Body Divide in Large Language Models and in Human Learners. Open Mind: Discoveries in Cognitive Science, 8, 84–101.
Berent, I. (2024, in press). Consciousness isn’t “hard”—it’s human psychology that makes it so! Neuroscience of Consciousness.
Berent, I (November, 27, 2024). Consciousness Has a Psychology Problem. Nautilus.
Berent, I., & Gervain, J. (2023). Speakers aren’t blank slates (with respect to sign-language phonology)! Cognition, 232, 105347.
Berent, I., Fried, P. J., Theodore, R. M., Manning, D., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2023).Phonetic categorization relies on motor simulation, but combinatorial phonological computations are abstract. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 874. doi:10.1038/s41598-023-28099-w
Nallet, C., Berent, I., Werker, J. & Gervain, J . (2023). The neonate brain’s sensitivity to repetition-based structure: specific to speech? Developmental Science, 00, e13408.
Berent, I. (2023). The illusion of the mind-body divide is attenuated in males. Scientific Reports, 13(1): p. 6653
Berent, I. (2023). The “hard problem of consciousness” arises from human psychology. Open Mind, 7 564–587.
Andan, Q., Bex, P., & Berent, I. (2023). Linguistic illusions guide eye movement: evidence from doubling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 52(52), 2811–2833.
Berent, I. (2023). How to tell a Dualist? Cognitive Science. 47(11), e13380.
Berent, I. and Platt, M. (2022) Berent, I. and Platt, M. (2022) Is Phonology Embodied? Evidence from Mechanical Stimulation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.
Berent, I., Vaknin-Nusbaum, V., & Galaburda, A. (2022). Phonology and phonetics in dyslexia—linked, but distinct: Evidence from dyslexia. Radical: a journal of phonology, 4, 491-527.
Berent (2020). Is intuitive psychology bad for psychology? Reply to Krueger. American Psychologist.
Berent I, Platt M, Theodore R, Balaban E, Fried PJ and Pascual-Leone A (2020) Speech Perception Triggers Articulatory Action: Evidence From Mechanical Stimulation. Front. Commun. 5:34. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2020.00034
Berent, I., Bat-el, O., Brentari, D., Platt, M. (2020) Knowledge of language transfers from speech to sign: Evidence from doubling. Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12809
Berent, I., Feldman Barrett, L., & Platt, M. (2020). Essentialists biases in reasoning about emotions. Frontiers In Psychology: Cognitive Science (23).
Berent, I. (2020). Empiricist intuitions arise from an ontological dissonance: Reply to Carruthers. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 27, 220-229.
Berent, I. & Marcus, G. (2019). No integration without structured representations: Reply to Pater. Language, 95, e75-e86.
Berent, 1., Platt, M., & Sandoboe, G. M. (2019). People’s intuitions about innateness. Accepted, pending minor revision, Open Mind: Discoveries in Cognitive Sciences. Advanced publication.
Berent, I. (2018). Is markedness a confused concept? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 493-499.
Andan, Q, Bat-El, O., Brentari, D., & Berent, I. (2018). ANCHORING is amodal: evidence from a signed language. Cognition, 180, 279-283
Berent, I & Dupuis, A. (2017). The unbounded productivity of (sign) language: Evidence from the Stroop task. The Mental Lexicon, 12, 309 –341
Zhao, X., & Berent, I. (2017). The basis of the syllable hierarchy: articulatory pressures or universal phonological constraints? . Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.
Berent, I, Bat-El, O & Vaknin-Nusbaum, V. (2017). The double identity of doubling: Evidence for the phonology/morphology split. Cognition.
Berent, I. (2017). On the origins of phonology. Current Directions in Psychological Science.
Zhao, X., & Berent, I. (2016). Universal Restrictions on Syllable Structure: Evidence From Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45, 795–811.
Berent, I. (2016). Evans’s (2014) modularity myths: A mental architecture digest. Language, 92, 195-197.
Berent, I. ( 2016). Commentary: An Evaluation of Universal Grammar and the phonological mind”—UG is still a viable hypothesis. Frontiers in Language Sciences.doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01029
Berent, I. & Dupuis, A. (2016). Where does (sign) language begin? Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
Berent, I., Zhao, X. , Balaban, E. & Galaburda, A. (2016). Phonology and phonetics dissociate in dyslexia evidence from adult English speakers Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31, 1178-1192.
Berent, I., Bat-El, O., Brentari, D., Dupuis, A., & Vaknin-Nusbaum, V. (2016). The double identity of linguistic doubling. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 13702-13707.
Dupuis, A., & Berent, I. (2015). Signs are symbols: evidence from the Stroop task. Language, Cognition, Neuroscience, 11, 1-6.
Berent, I., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). Language by mouth and by hand. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 78-78.
Berent, I., Brem, A.-K., Zhao, X., Seligson, E., Pan, H., Epstein, J., et al. (2015). Role of the motor system in language knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1983-1988.
Lennertz, T., & Berent, I. (2014). On the sonority levels of fricatives and stops. The Mental Lexicon, 10, 88–132.
Berent, I., Dupuis, A., & Brentari, D. (2014). Phonological reduplication in sign language: Rules rule. Frontiers in Language Sciences, 5, 560.
Berent, I. (2014). Rich languages from poor inputs ed. by Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini and Robert C. Berwick (review). Language, 90, 980-983.
Berent, I., Pan, H., Zhao, X., Epstein, J., Bennett, M. L., Deshpande, V., et al. (2014). Language universals engage Broca’s area.
Tamasi, K., & Berent, I. (2014). Sensitivity to phonological universals: The case of fricatives and stops. Journal of Psycholinguisitc research.
Gómez, D. M., Berent, I., Benavides-Varela, S., Bion, R. A. H., Cattarossi, L., Nespor, M., Mehler, J. (2014). Language universals at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
Berent, I., Vaknin-Nusbaum, V., Balaban, E., & Galaburda, A. (2013). Phonological generalizations in dyslexia: is the grammar impaired? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30: 285-310.
Berent, I. (2013). The phonological mind. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 17(7), 319-327.
Berent I, Dupuis A, Brentari D (2013) Amodal Aspects of Linguistic Design. PLoS ONE 8(4): e60617. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0060617.
Berent, I., Lennertz, T., & Rosselli, M. (2012). Universal linguistic pressures and their solutions: Evidence from Spanish. The Mental Lexicon, 13, 275-305.
Berent, I., Vaknin-Nusbaum, V., Balaban, E., & Galaburda, A., M., (2012). Dyslexia impairs speech recognition but can spare phonological competence. PLoS ONE, 7(9): p. e44875. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044875.
Berent, I. (in press). The phonological mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Berent, I., Wilson, C., Marcus, G., & Bemis, D. (2012). On the role of variables in phonology: Remarks on Hayes and Wilson. Linguistic Inquiry, 43.
Gervain, J., Berent, I., & Werker, J. (in press). Binding at birth: Newborns detect identity relations and sequential position in speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
Berent, I., Lennertz, T., & Smolensky, P. (2011). Markedness and misperception: It’s a two-way street. In C. E. Cairns & E. Raimy (Eds.), Handbook of the Syllable (pp. 373-394). Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill.
Berent, I. (2011). Core phonology: Evidence from grammatical universals. In L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Boston, MA: Cognitive Science Society.
Berent, I., Balaban, E., & Vaknin-Nusbaum, V. (2011). How linguistic chickens help spot spoken-eggs: phonological constraints on speech identification. Frontiers in Language Sciences, 2, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00182.
Berent, I., Harder, K., & Lennertz, T. (2011). Phonological universals in early childhood: Evidence from sonority restrictions. Language Acquistion, 18, 281–293.
Berent, I., Lennertz, T., & Balaban, E. (2011). Language universals and misidentification: A two way street.
Language and Speech, 1-20.
Berent, I., Balaban, E., Lennertz, T. & Vaknin-Nusbaum, V. (2010). Phonological universals constrain the processing of nonspeech stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 418-435.
Marom, M. & Berent, I. (2010). Phonological constraints on the assembly of skeletal structure in reading. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 67-88.
Berent, I., & Lennertz, T. (2010). Universal constraints on the sound structure of language: Phonological or acoustic? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 36, 212-223.
Lewkowicz, D. & Berent, I. (2009). Sequence Learning by 4 Month-Old Infants: Do infants represent ordinal information? Child Development, 80, 1811-1823.
Berent I. (2009). Unveiling phonological universals: A linguist who asks “why” is (inter alia) an experimental psychologist. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 450-451.
Berent, I., Lennertz, T., & Smolensky, P. (2009). Listeners’ knowledge of phonological universals: Evidence from nasal clusters. Phonology, 26, 75-108.
Berent, I., Lennertz, T., Jun, J., Moreno, M., A., & Smolensky, P. (2008). Language universals in human brains. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 5321-5325.
Berent, I, D. (2008). Are phonological representations of printed and spoken language isomorphic? Evidence from the restrictions on unattested onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 34, 1288-1304.
Berent, I., & Pinker, S. (2008). Compound formation is constrained by morphology: A reply to Seidenberg, MacDonald & Haskell. The Mental Lexicon, 3, 176-187.
Berent, I., & Pinker, S. (2007). The Dislike of Regular Plurals in Compounds: Phonological or Morphological? The Mental Lexicon, 2, 129-181.
Berent, I., Vaknin, V., & Marcus. G. (2007). Roots, stems, and the universality of lexical representations: Evidence from Hebrew. Cognition, 104, 254-286.
Berent, I., Lennertz, T, (2007). What we know about what we have never heard: Beyond Phonetics. Reply to Peperkamp. Cognition, 104, 638-643.
Berent, I., Steriade, D., Lennertz, T & Vaknin, V. (2007). What we know about what we have never heard: Evidence from perceptual illusions. Cognition, 104, 591-630.
Berent, I., Tzelgov, J. , & Bibi, U. (2006). The autonomous computation of morphophonological structure in reading: Evidence from the Stroop task. The Mental Lexicon, 1-2, 201-230.
Berent, I., Pinker, S., Tzelgov, J., Bibi, U., & Goldfarb, L. (2005). Computation of Semantic Number from Lexical, Morphological, and Conceptual Information. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 342-358.
Berent, I. & Marom, M. (2005). The skeletal structure of printed words: Evidence from the Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 328-338.
Berent, I., Vaknin, V. & Shimron, J. (2004). Does a theory of language need a grammar? Evidence from Hebrew root structure. Brain and Language, 90 (1-3), 170-182.
Marcus, G. F., & Berent, I. (2003). Are there limits to statistical learning? Science, 300, 53-55.
Berent, I. & Van Orden, G. C. (2003). Do null phonemic masking effects reflect strategic control of phonology? Reading and Writing, 16(4), 349-376.
Berent, I. & Shimron, J. (2003). Co-occurrence restrictions on identical consonants in the Hebrew lexicon: Are they due to similarity? Journal of Linguistics, 39(1), 31-55.
Berent, I. (2002). [Review of the book The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism and cognitive science, by Gary F. Marcus]. Language, 78(3), 569-571.
Berent, I., Pinker, S.& Shimron, J. (2002). The nature of Regularity and Irregularity: Evidence from Hebrew Nominal Inflection. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(5), 459-502.
Berent, I., Marcus, G. F., Shimron, J.& Gafos, A. I. (2002). The scope of linguistic generalizations: evidence from Hebrew word formation. Cognition, 83(2), 113-39.
Berent, I., Shimron, J.& Vaknin, V. (2001). Phonological constraints on reading: Evidence from the Obligatory Contour Principle. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(4), 644-665.
Berent, I., Everett, D. L. & Shimron, J. (2001). Do phonological representations specify variables? Evidence from the obligatory contour principle. Cognitive Psychology, 42(1), 1-60.
Berent, I., Bouissa, R. & Tuller, B. (2001). The effect of shared structure and content on reading nonwords: evidence for a CV skeleton. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(4), 1042-57.
Berent, I. (2001). Can connectionist models of phonology assembly account for phonology? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(4), 661-76.
Berent I. Identity avoidance in the Hebrew lexicon: implications for symbolic accounts of word formation. Brain Lang. 2002 Apr-Jun;81(1-3):326-41.
Berent, I. & Van Orden, G. (2000). Homophone dominance modulates the phonemic-masking effect. . Scientific studies of reading, 42, 133-167.
Berent, I., Pinker, S. & Shimron, J. (1999). Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: Evidence for mental variables. Cognition, 72, 1-44.
Berent, I. (1997). Phonological priming in the lexical decision task: Regularity effects are not necessary evidence for assembly. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1-16.
Berent, I. & Shimron, J. (1997). The representation of Hebrew words: Evidence from the Obligatory Contour Principle. Cognition, 64, 39-72.
Berent, I. & Perfetti, C. A. (1995). A rose is a REEZ: The two cycles model of phonology assembly in reading English. Psychological Review, 102, 146-184.
Berent, I. & Perfetti, C. A. (1993). An on-line method in studying music parsing. Cognition, 46, 203-222.